F
9507-PAR PARADIGM SHIFT in Language : transitivity in Halliday and Crombie paper by
Zachār Laskewicz ŠNIGHTSHADES PRESS 2008 music-theatre-language ebooks Noordstraat 1/3, 9000 This paper remains the original copyright of Zachār
Alexander LASKEWICZ who wrote this paper in 1996. If use is made of the
contents of this work, please reference the work appropriately and inform the
author at the following address : zachar@nachtschimmen.eu Reference
Code : 9507-PAR Correlating
PDF document: http://www.nachtschimmen.eu/_pdf/9507_PAR.pdf Introduction The grammatical approaches presented by
Halliday and Crombie form part of a new paradigm in linguistics. Case grammar, which found its strongest
theoretical proponent in Halliday and his 'transitivity', is an expression of
this paradigm in which the emphasis is placed back on the meaning-based
function of words and sentences rather than on a structuralistic
'transformative grammar' originating with Chomsky in the fifties. This is
directly expressed by a method of clause analysis in which the meaning-based
function of the words is used rather than a grammatical model based on the
abstract positioning of words within sentences, and this is where the term
'transitivity' needs to be extended. The
term is involved with the the role individual verb classes play in sentences,
meaning that the terminology requires considerably more than the traditional
definition allows: whether or not a verb requires an object. Halliday can assist us at this point. He says that "transitivity specifies the
different types of process that are recognized in the language, and the
structures by which they are expressed." (Halliday, 1985, pg. 101). Transitivity has revealed itself to be a
complex subject, as demonstrated by the different approaches taken by linguists
since Fillmore. In this assignment we
will be looking at two contrasting approaches to 'transitivity': that presented
by Halliday (1985), based on material and mental 'processes', and that of
Crombie (1985), based on dynamic, process and stative predicate
groups. Transitivity systems Before we begin with comparative
sentence analysis it is first important to clearly lay out the processes used
by Halliday and Crombie to help to
classify verb 'transitivity'. Halliday
defines the 'going-ons' of life as 'processes' which are represented in the
form of language. In terms of
traditional grammar, a 'process' can be considered that event or occurrence
that results from the use of a verb.
According to Halliday, such a 'process' consists of three components: (i) the process itself; (ii) participants in the process; (iii) circumstances associated with
the process. (Halliday, 1985, pg. 101). Halliday divides process types into two
major divisions, material and mental processes. Material
processes are defined as processes of 'doing': "they express the notion that
some entity 'does' something-which may be done 'to' some other entity."
(Halliday, 1985, pg. 103). These
material processes can be again divided into two major forms: dispositive
or 'doing to' and creative or 'bringing about'. e.g. The lion caught the tourist: dispositive The man built the house: creative. Another major distinction within the
material processes division is between event and action. An action is the direct occurrence performed
directly by the subject with material results: it something that is done. An event, however, is something that does not
have observable material result: it is something that happens. Compare the following two sentences: The lion sprang action The mayor resigned event Two further terms need to be classified
before we can move to the second process type which will be analysed in this
assignment: the names of the participants within material processes. The two participants are ACTOR (A) and
GOAL (G). Actor is the name given
to that element of a sentence which performs the process, and the goal is the
result of the action. As will be
demonstrated in the following example, the actor does not necessarily have to
be in the 'subject' position within the sentence, and its meaning is therefore
based on its function within the sentence rather than its position: The lion (A) caught the tourist (G). The tourist (G) was caught by the
lion (A). The second major type of process which
will be discussed in this assignment are what Halliday refers to as mental
processes. Halliday divides mental
processes into three major types: perception, affection and cognition. Perception processes are those involved with
the senses: smelling and seeing for example; affection processes involve the
relation the participant has with an object, whether that be an 'idea' or a
'thing' (liking, fearing, hating etc.); cognition processes involve what the
participant 'thinks' of a given object (thinking, knowing, understanding
etc.). Halliday assigns the names senser
and phenomenon to the two participants within a mental process
and affixes the following criteria to these processes: (i) there is always a human
participant as 'senser'; (ii) the phenomenon may be a 'thing' or a 'fact'; (iii) simple present is used as the
basis form- e.g. He is scared of ghosts. *He is
being scared of ghosts; (iv) the process can generally be
realized in both directions- e.g. he liked the book/the book pleased
him; (v) cannot be realized by 'do' or
happen'-
e.g. What did John do? *He liked the book; (Halliday, 1985, pp.108-111). In a similar manner, Crombie is
interested in the "semantic relationships that exist within propositions
[clauses]" (Crombie, 1985, pg. 95). In
order to avoid confusion at this point it may be useful to note terms that are
used by Crombie which have similar functions to those of Halliday but which are
named differently. Crombie refers to
'participants' as arguments and 'processes' as predicates. She notes three major types of predicates
which it will be revealed cross-over on one level or another with Halliday's
terminology. Her predicate types can be
distinguished as follows: Type 1: Dynamic predicates
(involving mental or physical activity) (a) general activity (e.g. write/eat) (b) Momentary activity (e.g.
nod/glance/wink) (c) Transitional event (e.g.
arrive/leave) (d) Mental activity (e.g.
choose/decide) (e) Factitive activity (an activity
which brings an entity into being) (e.g. build/construct) Type 2: Process predicates
(involving processes in which in which there is no active, conscious activity)
(e.g. deteriorate/boil/melt). Type 3:
Stative predicates (a) Inert perception and cognition
(e.g. understand/prefer/like) (b) Relational (e.g. own/consist
of/contain) (Crombie, 1985, pg. 98) In contrast to Halliday's participants
which are kept constant within each of the two categories, Crombie developed a
complex set of 'semantic roles' which differ depending on the predicate
type. She defines five major categories:
Causal roles, Participation roles, Orientation-transition roles, Relational
roles and the Abaxiant role.
These categories are involved with different classifications for the
'arguments' that specific predicate types can adopt. For example, Process predicates has
'process-participation roles' which are unique to the Process predicates
category. These Participation roles
include those roles which involve the "non-causal involvement of an entity or
abstraction in an activity or with a process or state." (Crombie, 1985, pg.
102). Within each of these categories
she defines for which type of predicate type the different roles can be
assigned. This is quite a complex set of
categories and distinctions, and at this stage it is safer to demonstrate them
by showing examples from Crombie's work
and contrasting them with those presented by Halliday. Comparisons The method within this paper is to take
a number of sentences used in Halliday's analysis and a number of sentences
used in Crombie's and analyse them in terms of the transitivity systems
presented by the other. Hopefully from
this analysis we will gain a clear idea of how the systems differ and in which
ways they are related to one another. Halliday examples -The lion caught the tourist. For Halliday, this is clearly a material
process. The lion is the actor
and the tourist the goal. In
addition the sentence is clearly an 'action' of the dispositive type, For Crombie this sentence adopts a Dynamic
predicate , which can be subdivided into the 'general activity'
category. Here we can refer to Crombie's
'semantic roles': the lion is the agent (from the causal roles), and the
tourist is the patient (from the participation roles). -The lion sprang. Halliday would refer to this sentence
again as a material process. According
to Halliday's model, goals need not be involved in the process, so 'the lion'
becomes the solo actor. Again the
sentence falls into the category 'action' of the dispositive type. Crombie would define the sentence again
as a Dynamic predicate, although her terminology would place the
sentence into the 'momentary activity' category. Like Halliday, 'the lion' would again become
the agent. -The mayor resigned. Halliday would refer to this sentence
as a material process, but this time he would refer to it as an
'event'. The mayor is again the actor. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into the dynamic predicate category, and the mayor as the agent. -A new approach is evolving. Halliday would refer to this sentence
as a material process falling into the second category 'event'. 'A new approach' is the actor and 'is evolving'
is the process itself. In contrast with Halliday's definition,
Crombie would define this sentence as belonging to type 2: Process
predicates. This involves the
assigning of new semantic roles. 'A new
approach' falls into the participation roles category and takes the name Mutant
(Mu): the entity that is changed by the process (in this case of evolving). -Children fear ghosts. Halliday would refer to this sentence
as a mental process of the affection type. The 'children' take the participant role of the
'senser' and the ghosts take the role of the 'phenomenon'. Crombie would place this sentence into
type 3: Stative predicates, category (a) Inert perception and
cognition. Again, contrasting
semantic roles are assigned. Both
categories come from the 'participation roles'.
'Children' are defined as being the Experiencer (E), and the
ghost are defined as being the Appertainant (Ap), which is defined as
being an "entity or abstraction experienced in a particular
way." This can be compared to Halliday's
criteria for mental processes in which the phenomenon is allowed to be either a
'thing' or a 'fact'. -Do you know the city? Halliday would refer to this sentence
as a mental process, falling into the cognition type. The participants are again senser and
phenomenon, taken by 'you' and 'the city' respectively. Crombie would place this sentence again
into type 3: Stative predicates, category (a) Inert perception and
cognition. 'You' is the Experiencer
and the 'the city' is the Appertainant.
It is interesting to note that Crombie places it into the same
category, whereas as Halliday has a different way to subdivide this particular
level and so has a separate category for this type of process. -[If there was anything out there] we'd hear it coming. Halliday would refer to this sentence
as a mental process, falling into the perception type. 'We' is the senser and 'it coming' is phenomenon
(in this case, a fact). Crombie would again place this sentence
into type 3: Stative predicates, category (a) Inert perception and
cognition, where 'we' is the Experiencer, and 'it coming' is the Appertainant. Here the three mental processes of Halliday
(perception, affection and cognition) are contrasted with the single
possibility available within the subdivisions provided by Crombie. Crombie examples -I smell petrol. Crombie would place this sentence into
type 3: Stative predicate, category (a) Inert perception and
cognition. 'I' is the Experiencer
and 'petrol' is the Appertainant. Halliday would define this sentence as
a mental process of the perception type, with 'I' taking the participant
role of senser and 'petrol' of phenomenon. -I am smelling petrol. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into type 1: Dynamic predicates, category (a) General
activity. 'I' is the Agent
and 'petrol' is the Patient. The fact that Crombie would change the
category because of this type of contrast in meaning is different to the way
Halliday would characterise the sentence.
He doesn't have a list of distinctions that refer to whether a sentence
occurs directly or over a period of time. Halliday would define the sentence
again as a mental process of the perception type, with 'I' taking the
participant role of senser and
'petrol' of phenomenon. -The mechanic repaired the car. Crombie would place this sentence into
type 1: Dynamic predicates, category (a) General activity. 'The mechanic' would take the role of Agent
and 'the car' of Patient. Halliday would define this sentence as
a material process of the 'action' type.
'The mechanic' is the actor and 'the car' is the goal. -The butter melted. Crombie would place the sentence into
type 2: Process predicates. The
semantic roles involved contrast again, giving us the term Mutant which
is the entity changed by the process. This precision of definition contrasts
to Halliday's system which would simply refer to it as a material
process. The butter is defined as being
the actor according to what is referred to as the 'ergative function':
"Halliday [. . .] defines this function in terms of an affected participant
which is the one inherent role associated with action clauses, and which is the
goal in a transitive and the actor in an intransitive clause." (Kennedy, 1982,
pg. 85). -The sun melted the butter. Crombie would place the sentence again
into type 2: Process predicates.,
where 'the sun' takes a new semantic role of Force and 'the butter' is
again the Mutant. A 'force' is
described as a "non-sentient causative which precludes the explicit or implicit
involvement of an agent" (Crombie, 1985, pg. 101). Halliday, in not having the same amount
of semantic roles to assign to the participants of this sentence, would refer
to it again as being a material process in which 'the sun' is the actor and
'the butter' is the goal. The sentence
is of course an 'event' because something is 'happening' rather than being
directly 'done'. -The plant grew. Crombie would place this sentence into
type 2: Process predicates, where 'the plant' becomes the Mutant. Halliday would refer to the sentence as a material
process and an 'event'. Because of the
ergative function, the plant would be defined as the actor. -The boy grew tired. Crombie would place this sentence into
type 2: Process predicates. The
boy is placed again into the semantic role of Mutant. The 'predicate' itself is defined as falling
into the Material Processes category. (Crombie, 1985, pg. 85). Halliday would define the sentence as a
material process, and an 'event'. the
boy is defined as being the actor because of the ergative function. -The coin rolled down the hill. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into type 1: Dynamic predicates, type (a) General activity. Contrasting semantic roles would be
assigned. 'The coin' would become the Object,
which Crombie defines as "the entity described as being in a particular
location or as being involved in a transitional event." (Crombie, 1985, pg.
103). 'the hill' would be assigned the
role of Range, which is defined by Crombie as being "the location of a
static entity or the path or area traversed by a moving entity." (Crombie,
1985, pg. 103). For Halliday, the sentence would simply
be a material process, and an 'event'.
'The coin' would be the actor and the phrase 'down the hill'
would be defined as being a 'circumstance associated with the process'. This contrasts to Crombie's more precise
terminology for each of the roles played within the sentence. -The music was heard by the prince. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into type 3: Stative predicates, type (a) Inert perception
and cognition. 'The prince' is the
Experiencer and the 'the music' is the Appertainant. Halliday would define this sentence as
being a mental process of the perception type. 'The music' is the phenomenon and 'the
prince' is the senser. This sentence demonstrates that this approach
to grammar in both cases places semantic/participant roles based not on the
position within the sentence but on the actual function of the words. In this passive form, the roles stay with the
same words as when the sentence is in its active form: The prince heard the
music. The passive form does not
change the essential roles assigned to the words. -He died of Polio. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into type 1: Dynamic predicates.
'Polio' takes the causative semantic role of Force, whereas 'he'
becomes the Patient. The sentence could be rewritten in the
following way: Polio killed him. In this
sense, Halliday would define the sentence as being a material process
and an 'event', in which 'polio' is the actor and 'he' is the goal. -He made models from matches. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into type 1: Dynamic predicates, category (e) Factitive. This category brings a number of new semantic
roles with it. 'He' is the Agent, 'models' is the Result and
'from matches' is the Material. These
roles are connected only to factitative predicate types, and are absent
in the Halliday material/mental processes model. Halliday would define this sentence as
being a material process and an 'event'.
'He' would be defined as the actor and 'models' would be defined
as the goal of the process, whereas 'from matches' would be defined as
the 'circumstance associated with the process.' -He dreamed a dream about the sea. Crombie would define this sentence as
falling into type 3: Stative predicates, category (a) Inert
perception and cognition. 'He' would
be defined as the Experiencer, and 'a dream about the sea' would be
defined as the Appertainant. Halliday would define this sentence as
being a mental process of the 'cognitive' type. 'He' would be the senser and 'a dream
about the sea' would be the phenomenon. -John boiled the milk. Crombie would place this sentence into
type 1: Dynamic predicates, category (a) General activity where'John'
is the Agent . This sentence is
viewed however as being of a 'double propositional' nature which can be
rewritten as follows: John caused it (the milk boiled), in which 'the milk' is
viewed as the Mutant: the entity which is changed by a process. Halliday would define this sentence as
being a material process and an 'action'. 'John' would be the actor and 'the
milk' would be the goal. Conclusions It could be said that the models for
the analysis of verb functions within clauses presented by Crombie and Halliday
coincide on some levels. For example,
Halliday uses the terms Senser and Phenomenon within his mental
processes. Crombie, as a subdivision of
the Process predicates type (a) Inert perception and cognition,
two semantic roles can be assigned: Experiencer and Appertainant. These categories are practically the same,
made even more clearly be Halliday's recognition of both 'things' and 'ideas'
as types of phenomenon , just as Crombie recognises that the semantic
role of Appertainant can be taken by an 'entity' or an
'abstraction'. We can name further areas
of coincidence. Halliday's mental
processes allow for an actor and a goal, which can be directly
compared to Crombie's Agent and Patient. If we observe the contrasts, however,
they seem to far outweigh the similarities.
Halliday has distinctions and categories that are not present in
Crombie's system, just as Crombie distinguishes things which are not recognized
within Halliday's system. Halliday, for
example, in his mental process distinction recognises three contrasting
verbal types: perception, affection and cognition. This system is absent in Crombie's, in which
a single category within the division Stative predicates: type (a) Inert
perception and cognition is allowed for.
Crombie's system, however, in its recognition of many different
types of 'semantic roles', makes a lot of classification that is completely
unaccounted for in Halliday's version of transitivity. Crombie's Process predicates, for
example, has a semantic role that is not considered by Halliday: Mutant. Crombie's 'factitive' Dynamic
predicates has two semantic roles not included in Halliday's system: Material
and Result. Crombie's complex
list of 'semantic roles' in which elements within a sentence are given very particular
and definable functions contrasts with Halliday's much less complicated
'participants' which are completely dependent on the process type. Other contrasts also present themselves. Crombie has a category which distinguishes
between Dynamic predicates which occur 'momentarily' as opposed to those
which occur as a 'general activity'. This contrast is further demonstrated when
we observe Crombie's tendency to distinguish between 'stative' and 'dynamic'
predicates which actually would remain in the same category according to
Halliday (e.g. I smell the petrol, I am smelling the petrol). Halliday's use of the mental processes
is completely confused when we observe that Crombie's Dynamic predicates
have a mental activity subdivision, and that also within the Stative
predicates type we observe the Inert
perception and cognition subdivision.
In this case, sentences that can be classified into the one category in
Halliday's system have to span two completely different predicate types in
Crombie's system. This is, however, a question of the
division of semantic space. Halliday has
chosen a different emphasis to that of Crombie, and so some elements are
emphasised by the one and left in a simplified form by the other. Although
there are more contrasts than similarities between the two systems it is
possible to conclude that they are united in the fact that they are involved in
a particular way of looking at grammar.
Here the verbs are viewed as being a complex element within the sentence
which determines the structure of the involved clause; here 'participants'
(Halliday) or 'arguments' (Crombie) are involved directly in a verbal
'process'. The meaning of these
'processes' are related directly to the dynamics involved between the
participants and the process itself, and both Crombie and Halliday have
attempted to find categories which can help us to define how these verbal
systems actually work. References Crombie, W. (1985). Process and
relation in Discourse and language learning (pp. 95-114). Halliday, M. A. K. (1985). An
introduction to functional grammar. Holmes, J. & Brown, D. G.
(1987). Teachers and students
learning about compliments. TESOL
Quarterly, 21 (3): 523-546. Kennedy, C. (1982). Systemic grammar
and its use in literary analysis. In Ronald Carter (ed.) Language and
literature: An introductory reader in stylistics (pp. 83-99). London:
Allen and Unwin.
Š May 2008 Nachtschimmen
Music-Theatre-Language Night Shades,
Ghent (Belgium)*
|
|
Major Writings
|
|